Longitudinal Data
Longitudinal data analysis is a subspecialty of stat-istics in which individual histories—interpreted as sample paths,or realizations,of a stochastic process—are the primary focus of interest.A wide variety of scientific questions can only be addressed by utilizing longitudinal data together with statistical methods that facilitate the detection and characterization of regularities across multiple individual histories.De-spite the stated focus on individual histories,most of the analytical strategies either invoke strong assump-tions about stochastic dependencies across time and\ or use variable-centered methods to study the influence of multiple covariates on process dynamics and its relationship to diverse outcomes.An exception to this pattern is the literature on growth curve analysis, where the full individual histories are modeled and frequently aggregated into categories of similar his-tories.This set of techniques,as currently constituted, has severe limitations when a study objective is to understand the interrelationships between multiple dimensions of a multivariate time series.Problems arise due to the utilization of interaction terms,as in regression analyses,which represent the imposition of a variable-centered idea in an inherently person-centered question.
The purpose of this entry is to describe some person-centered methods that respond to the challenge of characterizing life histories,ascertained with longi-tudinal data,placing emphasis on the effective util-izati
on of both cross-time and multiple-life-domain information.We motivate the methods and illustrate their use in the context of explaining how a variety of health outcomes occur.However,the methodological issues are generic and transfer to a wide range of problems in the behavioral and social sciences.
1.Moti ation
Extensive empirical evidence documents associations between early and mid-life risk factors and later-life chronic disease and disability(Barker and Osmond 1986,Wadsworth and Kuh1997).Thesefindings generate considerable interest in understanding the subtle nuances of life histories that might explain how such outcomes occur.Broad-gauged interest in suc-cessful aging(Rowe and Kahn1998),with particular emphasis on how some individuals manage to ad-judicate effectively multiple difficult challenges,also suggests in-depth comparative studies of life histories. Detailed accounts of whole lives have previously been studied primarily as individual cases,with evidence presented in forms ranging from rich ethnographies to focused interviews and,in a medical context,from open-ended responses to structured inquiries by clinicians.
Complementing such narrative accounts of life histories are structured longitudinal surveys that contain—for each individual—hundreds,or even thousands,of responses to questions about experi-enc
es over time and across multiple life domains (Hauser et al.1993,Power et al.1991,Wadsworth and Kuh1997).In principle,those interested in the impact of multiple facets of life histories on a given outcome(s) should be able to construct representations of whole lives from the survey responses.Despite considerable interest in this task across multiple disciplines(Elder 1974,Vaillant1983),there has been a dearth of analytic strategies for producing representations of whole lives and for aggregating them into meaningful taxonomies that facilitate understanding of how given outcomes occur.
With these observations at hand,our primary objective is to describe top-down and bottom-up methods for constructing taxonomies of life history representations and use them as the basis for explain-ing how given outcomes occur.We will also emphasize the construction of narratives from numerical survey responses and their utilization as part of an analytical strategy to understand‘whole lives.’We illustrate the ideas and methods in the context of health outcomes. However,the methodological issues are quite general and by no means restricted to this class of outcomes.
2.Classes of Methods
The techniques discussed in this entry are called person-centered in the sense that the individual life h
istory is the unit of analysis.Within this broad class of methods are two categories of techniques called ‘bottom-up’and‘top-down.’The bottom-up strategies begin with highly idiosyncratic individual histories, and the analytical steps then identify important commonalities and differences across lives,leading to aggregation of histories into relatively homogeneous groups.Optimal matching(Abbott and Hrycak1990), p-technique integrated with dynamic factor analysis (Shifren et al.1997),and the integrated qualitative–quantitative aggregation strategy of Singer et al.(1998) are examples of bottom-up strategies.
Top-down techniques sequentially partition a het-erogeneous population into progressively more homo-geneous sub-groups.Recursive partitioning(Zhang and Singer1999),Grade-of-Membership models (Berkman et al.1989),and the clustering methods of Bergman(1998)are generic examples of these methods.
3.A Bottom-up Example:
Understanding Psychological Resilience
3.1The Problem
Among those who experience major episodes of depression in adult life,or earlier,some manage to
9058 Longitudinal Data
subsequently regain high levels of well-being—that is, they demonstrate resilience.We ask whether those who show recovery have particular features that distinguish them from those who continue to suffer depression.An affirmative answer could suggest pre-vention strategies which,if applied early in life,might serve to bypass episodes of depression,or minimize their severity and facilitate recovery from them.
3.2Longitudinal Data
The Wisconsin Longitudinal Study(WLS)began with a random sample of10,317men and women who graduated from Wisconsin high schools in1957. Survey data were collected from the original re-spondents in1957,1975,and1992\93.Telephone interviews were conducted in1992\93with8,020of the1975respondents,6,535of whom also responded to a mail survey.Data have been collected on respondents’family background,starting resources, academic abilities,youthful aspirations,social sup-port,social comparisons,and the timing and se-quencing of adult educational and occupational achievements,work events and conditions,family events,and physical and mental health.We focus on a subset of the5,009primary respondents who partici-pated in all three waves of the WLS and who respo
nded to questions assessing affective disorder and well-being in the most recent survey.The affective questions were administered to a randomly selected80 percent of respondents.For each individual,there were responses on over one thousand variables cover-ing all three waves of the survey and information across the multiple life domains indicated above. The resilience criteria(see Singer et al.1998)were satisfied by168women.There were59women who satisfied the depressed\unwell criteria.The analytical task is the development of a taxonomy of life histories for each group such that the characterization of any history is sufficiently nuanced to capture the richness of individual experience,but not so detailed as to represent only very few women.For a more broad-based discussion of resilience,using both narrative and numerical data on individual histories to the study of racial and ethnic inequalities in health,see Singer and Ryff(1997).
3.3A Person-centered Strategy
Construction of an empirical taxonomy of life histories requires a set of organizing principles—based on prior literature—that constrain the distillation of over one thousand responses for each individual into composite variables.Such composites are the basis for the ultimate representations of categories of histories.The followingfive principles were used to specify combi-nations of individual characteristics essential to explain how the outcomes come about.
(a)Adversity and its cumulation over time has negative mental health consequences.
(b)Advantage and its cumulation over time has positive mental health consequences.
(c)Reactions to adversity or advantage influence the impact of life experiences.
(d)Position in social hierarchies has consequences for mental health.
(e)Social relationships influence the impact of life experiences.
These propositions are not new to the social sciences,mental health,or agingfields.What is new is the integration of them into an analytical strategy to guide life-history analysis.The methodology is use-fully decomposed intofive principle steps:
(1)guided by the organizing principles,life history narratives are constructed for a sub-sample of cases from each outcome ,resilient and depressed\unwell);
(2)abstracted chronological charts,each one repre-senting the synthesis of several narratives,were constructed;
(3)reduced response vectors,b,consisting of com-posite variables suggested by the chronological charts and individual variables whose rationale derives from the organizing principles are specified;
(4)aggregates(categories)of histories,in terms of response vectors judged to be close to one another,are specified;and
(5)tests of distinguishability are conducted.
Step(5)reduces to ascertaining whether or not the categories of histories characterizing the resilient women occur with low frequency in the depressed\ unwell group and,conversely,whether histories char-acterizing the depressed\unwell women occur with low frequency among the resilient women.If the taxonomies of histories are to be a basis for ex-planation of the mental health outcomes,there should be distinguishability of categories of histories between the two groups.
3.4Narrati es
A small random sample("10cases)is selected from each outcome group,and a narrative biographical story is written about each individual.We adopt Stone’s(1979)conception of narrative,defined as‘the organization of material in a chronological sequential order and the focusing of the content into a singl
e coherent story,albeit with subplots.’Our rationale for writing narratives based on survey data(with over one thousand responses per individual)is that the human mind has difficulty processing enormous amounts of information in ,long lists of responses representing a single life),although it is well suited to process a coherent ,a written narrative about a life)(Turner1996).The use of narratives here is qualitatively different from their role when they are part of the raw data.Specifically,the construction of
reactions to the online manage
9059
Longitudinal Data
narratives—by the analyst and starting with numerical response data—is part of an analytical strategy.The narratives do not represent empirical data derived from focused interviews,recorded conversations,or text written by the respondents,but stories written by investigators to describe‘whole lives through time.’In generating the narratives,responses selected for in-clusion were those expected to impact mental health, based on prior theoretical and empirical literature. The goal is to tell a life story in such a way that omitted information would make little or no difference for understanding the relevant life experiences preceding and predicting midlife mental health outcomes.
3.5Abstract Chronological Charts
Two life histories can be aggregated and called similar, or even approximately indistinguishable,when the response vectors representing them are similar.At the level of individual cases,represented by raw response data on more than one thousand variables or by narratives,there is so much idiosyncratic detail that meaningful aggregation seems almost hopeless.How-ever,via the organizing principles,it is possible to start with three or four narratives and begin specifying composite variables that ultimately lead to the speci-fication of response vectors,some of which are sufficiently similar that they can be aggregated into a common category.
The composite variables are the initial basis for revised response vectors for all individuals that need to satisfy two criteria.First,the revised response vector for a given individual should provide for a nuanced summary of the person’s life history as it relates to mental health outcomes.Second,the response vector should not be too idiosyncratic.The latter condition is essential if we are to aggregate life histories—represented as reduced response vectors—so that individuals with‘similar’response vectors can be grouped into common categories of histories.
3.6Reduced Response Vectors
Guided by the abstract chronological charts,we specify a provisional reduced vector of variables, whose values for a single individual define the person’s reduced response vector.The provisional reduced vector of variables will,clearly,represent all persons for whom we have constructed narratives.However, this is only a small sub-sample of a much larger population.We augment this group with10–15new cases for whom we do not have narratives.Their provisional reduced response vectors augmented by responses on additional variables that are clearly linked to the organizing principles are displayed. Visual examination of these augmented response vectors for the new cases AND those for which there are narratives serves to suggest new composite vari-ables and revisions of those specified for the pro-visional reduced vector of variables.The result of this visual inspection and judgment is a new second-stage reduced vector of variables.We continue bringing in blocks of10–15new cases at a time,repeating the above step,and thereby iteratively refining the reduced vector of response variables until all cases for a given mental health outcome group have been examined. Thefinal reduced vector of response variables,b,is the basis for initiating the formal specification of a taxonomy of histories.
3.7Aggregation of Reduced Response Vectors: Pathway Specification
All variables are identified by binary codes in b.Next, we identify the longest AND statement common t
o at least20individuals.This identifies a set of response vectors—equivalently,a set of life histories—that exactly match one another on the components of the AND statement.If the components of the AND statement are distributed across the whole lives and they are interpretable as meaningful pathways to a given mental health outcome,then we have succeeded in aggregating a set of life histories into a well-defined group.For the resilient women in WLS,the binary coded version of b had17components,and the longest AND statement common to at least20individuals had seven components.
With an initial aggregate of histories at hand,we eliminate this group from further consideration and identify the longest possible AND statement char-acterizing at least20—and hopefully more—of the remaining men.In the WLS resilient women,this did not lead to a long enough AND statement that was sufficiently nuanced to characterize possible routes into episodes of depression,as well as the cumulation of advantage that would promote re-covery.This led to specification of more complex Boolean statements that were both consistent with our organizing principles and represented the life histories of a substantial number of women.At least two strategies can be used for this specification.First, visual inspection of the17-component response vectors revealed a group of48women,all of whom grew up in a household with at least one alcoholic parent AND who had a dive
rsity of different forms of compensating advantage in adulthood(represented by logical OR statements).For a narrative description of the resulting Boolean statements,see Singer et al. (1998).
A more formally mechanized approach to this step—using,for example,Ragin’s QCA software (1987)—would be to minimize the Boolean expression representing all women who are not in thefirst group identified via the‘longest possible AND statement criterion.’The resulting minimal Boolean expression would,hopefully,have clearly interpretable com-ponents that would represent distinct pathways—
9060 Longitudinal Data
hence,additional members of the desired taxonomy of life histories—to given mental health outcomes. Although the analysis in Singer et al.(1998)proceeded beyond thefirst specification of an initial group by inspection and judgment,linked to organizing prin-cipals,we recommend application of both strategies en route to afinal taxonomy.Indeed,as illustrated in Ragin(1987),minimal Boolean expressions that are not readily interpretable on afirst pass through reduced response vectors often suggest recoding and construction of alternative composite variables that ultimately lead to a useful taxonomy.
3.8Tests of Distinguishability
Having constructed a taxonomy of pathways for each outcome category—resilient and depressed\unwell in the present example—it is necessary to demonstrate that the routes to any one outcome differ in important ways from the set of routes to a different outcome.To this end,wefirst ask whether the aggregate pathways for the resilient women differ from the pathways for the depressed\unwell population.In particular,we tabulate the frequency with which the resilient path-ways occur in the depressed\unwell population.Con-versely,we take the aggregate pathways representing the depressed\unwell women and tabulate the fre-quency with which those pathways actually occur among the resilient women.We expect tofind that the resilient pathways are rare in the depressed\unwell population and also that the depressed\unwell path-ways are rare in the resilient population.For example, 16percent of the resilient women are on the pathway indicated above by the seven-component logical AND statement.Only8percent of the depressed\unwell have the same pathway(p .10).Twenty percent of the resilient women are on the pathway where at least one parent is a chronic alcoholic AND there is a diversity of compensating positive experiences in adulthood.This pathway only occurs for5percent of the depressed\unwell population(p .05).There are persistently negative-experience pathways in the depressed\unwell population that have no counter-part in the resilient group.Sharper discrimination occurs depending upon the particular application(,Zhao et al.2000).The central point,however,is that assessment of distinguishability is important for making th
e case that there are,as hypothesized, qualitatively different routes to distinct mental health outcomes.
4.An Alternati e Bottom-up Strategy
An analytical strategy that might serve as an alterna-tive to what is put forth in the above example is optimal matching(Abbott and Hrycak1990,Abbott and Barman1997).Optimal matching derives from the literature on comparison of DNA sequences.It takes univariate time series for individuals and groups them together into sets of what are regarded as equivalent histories.The grouping requires the avail-ability of a metric on the space of possible sequences (histories);and two sequences are defined to be members of the same group if their distance apart—in terms of the metric—is sufficiently small.To apply the automated optimal matching technique requires that input data be in the form of what we have called ‘reduced response vectors.’The analogue of steps leading from more complex longitudinal data to this stage,as we have described above,must still be carried out for univariate time series prior to carrying out the formal aggregation step(Abbott and Hrycak1990). Although optimal matching is a natural way to proceed in the construction of a taxonomy of histories, the problem with its implementation on cross-time, multi-life-domain data is that there is no obvious metric yet developed that is suited to the complex multidimensional time series corresponding to indi-vidual histories,as discussed in the above example. Furthermore,a single measure of distance between two multidimensi
onal histories seems too coarse for decisions about what constitute equivalent histories. The strategy for aggregation of reduced response vectors—in the above example—uses information about multiple features of life histories.This suggests that any automated aggregation technique,analogous to optimal matching,should be based on a multi-dimensional measure on the space of histories.
5.Top-down Methods
There is a considerable diversity of top-down,person-centered analytical strategies.A valuable review of both concepts and methods is contained in Cairns et al.(1998).One common strategy focuses on clustering techniques,where a critical input to the algorithms is a metric on the space of histories.Not surprisingly, applications of these techniques,which partition a heterogeneous population into relatively homo-geneous clusters,have been restricted to low-dimensional data and rather simple summaries of histories.To reach the level of nuance and multi-domain representation of the kind we have discussed above requires a quite different set of measures of similarity of histories to be constructed than here-tofore.This is an important research topic for the future.
Recursive partitioning(RP)classification(Breiman et al.1984,Zhang and Singer1999)is an automated procedure thatfits trees to multi-dimensional longi-tudinal data such that pathways down the tree to a given outcome(terminal node)identify homogeneous sub-populations with common histories.Trees are
9061
Longitudinal Data
constructed with the objective of maximizing ability to predict membership in the outcome categories.Each pathway down the tree ending in a given terminal node (identified,for example,with a health outcome) corresponds to a logical AND statement incorporating information across multiple life domains.Two or more pathways into the same terminal node are identified with a logical OR statement describing how that particular outcome occurred.The judgment of the investigator is required to determine whether all pathways to the nodes identified with a given outcome should be summarized by a single complex Boolean expression,or partitioned into two or more sets of pathways defining qualitatively different kinds of histories.Recursive partitioning(RP)algorithms could accept either the original raw response vectors or reduced response vectors,as described in the above example.
From our perspective,a limitation of RP for generating a taxonomy of life histories is the fact that the performance criteria and the splitting rules for tree construction are not constrained by substantive knowledge from anyfield of scientific ,a set of organizing principles).Thus,pathways down the best predictive tree are not always interpretable within the knowledge of a givenfield.This disjunctio
n between statistical performance criteria,based solely on numerical goodness-of-fit,and constraints on tree structure and splitting rules derived from extant scientific knowledge have proven problematic in past applications of RP(Levy et al.1981,1985),for example,applied RP to neurological assessments of comatose patients to predict which patients were likely to recover with moderate disabilities as opposed to remaining vegetative or dying within one year of hospital admission.The numerically best predictive trees were virtually never neurologically interpretable. Rather,inspection of20j nearly optimal trees led to selection,based on scientific judgment,of a prognostic tree that had interpretable pathways.For a start at more interactive—i.e.,allowing the investigator to modify trees during the construction process on the basis of organizing principles—andflexible strategies based on RP technology,the reader should consult Zhang and Singer(1999).
6.Conclusions
We have described a variety of person-centered methods for the analysis of longitudinal data.Par-ticular emphasis has been given to a relatively new strategy—the bottom-up example—that takes a full complex individual history as the unit of analysis.The principal and novel features of this strategy are: (a)It includes the writing of narratives at the beginning and terminal stages of the analysis of survey data.
(b)It is person-not variable-centered.
(c)It produces pathways that represent aggregates of individual life histories.
(d)It works simultaneously across multiple life domains.
(e)It is capable of identifying multiple pathways to a given outcome.
(f)It systematizes the decision making en route to delineation of the pathways.
(g)It tests hypotheses about whole lives that are the integration of what is typically studied as separate pieces.
Point(a)underscores the importance of examining whole lives in story form as a means of comprehending the volume of detail that necessarily comprises human lives.Although point(b)is a clear objective in much of the life-history ,Elder1974,Magnusson and Bergman1990),the lack of person-centered methodological tools has prevented investigators from proceeding to point(c).Attempts to get at aggregates have typically involved the use of variable-centered techniques,since these were available.However,the investigators mentioned above and many others were well aware that the lack of preservation of individuals as units of analysis severely limited the infe
rences they could draw about the dynamics of individual(or aggregates of individuals)change.Point(d)is con-ventionally dealt with,if at all,by variable-centered techniques.The bottom-up example strategy is,to the best of our knowledge,thefirst incorporation of multiple life domain information in a process of aggregation of whole life representations.Many ,Harris et al.1990,Magnusson and Bergman1990)have struggled with point(e);however, the absence of tools for aggregating whole life rep-resentations has prevented their delineating full path-ways.Harris et al.(1990)set forth a template of several pathways to depression,and Magnusson and Bergman (1990)looked for‘patterns’connecting childhood problems with adult maladjustment.Nevertheless, their variable-centered empirical analyses(using logis-tic regression with interaction terms in the former,and cluster analysis in the latter)did not achieve the desired objective.What was needed was a person-centered methodology.Point(f)emphasizes the criti-cal role of scientific judgment embedded in the progression of methodological steps,something that is an essential part of any data analysis,but is in-frequently reported.Finally,point(g)carries hypothesis testing into the uncharted territory of whole lives.
Recommended reading for more detail on the person-centered methods emphasized in this entry and on the integration of numbers and narratives to study whole lives,including narratives as data,see Sing
er et al.(1998),Singer and Ryff,(2001),and Sampson and Laub(1993).An in-depth exposition of recursive partitioning,including online software,is contained in Zhang and Singer(1999).An instructive example of formal modeling of multiple-episode event history
9062 Longitudinal Data
data is given in Heckman and Walker(1998)and a useful illustration of modeling of multivariate event history data,with both observed and unobserved covariates,is contained in Manton et al.(1987).
See also:Adulthood:Developmental Tasks and Cri-tical Life Events;Factor Analysis and Latent Structure: Overview;Growth Curve Analysis;Life Management, Developmental Psychology of;Lifespan Develop-ment,Theory of;Linear Hypothesis:Regression (Basics);Longitudinal Data:Event–History Analysis in Discrete Time;Markov Models and Social Analysis; Person-centered Research
Bibliography
Abbott A,Barman E1997Sequence comparison via alignment and Gibbs sampling:A formal analysis of the emergence of the modern sociological article.In:Raftery A(ed.)Sociological Methodology,1997.American Sociological Association, Washington,DC,pp.47–87
Abbott A,Hrycak A1990Measuring resemblance in sequence data:An optimal matching analysis of musician’s careers. American Journal of Sociology96:144–85
Barker D,Osmond C1986Infant mortality,childhood nutrition, and ischaemic heart disease in England and Wales.Lancet I: 1077–81
Bergman L1998A pattern-oriented approach to studying individual development:Snapshots and processes.In:Cairns R,Bergman L,Kagan J(eds.)Methods and Models for Studying the Indi idual.Sage Publications,Thousand Oaks, CA,pp.83–122
Berkman L,Singer B,Manton K1989Black\white differences in health status and mortality among the elderly.Demography 26(4):661–78
Breiman L,Friedman J,Olshen R,Stone C1984Classification and Regression Trees.Wadsworth,Belmont,CA
Cairns R,Bergman L,Kagan J(eds.)1998Methods and Models for Studying the Indi idual.Sage Publications,Thousand Oaks, CA
Elder G1974Children of the Great Depression:Social Change in Life Experience.University of Chicago
Press,Chicago Harris T,Brown G,Bifulco A1990Loss of parent in childhood and adult psychiatric disorder:A tentative overall model. De elopment and Psychopathology2:311–28
Hauser R,Carr D,Hauser T,Hayes J,Krecker M,Kuo H, Magee W,Presti J,Shinberg,D,Sweeney M,Thompson-Colon T,Uhrig S Warren J1993The Class of1957After35 years:O er iew and Preliminary Findings.Working paper 93-17,Center for Demography and Ecology,University of Wisconsin-Madison,Madison,WI
Heckman J,Walker J1998Using goodness offit and other criteria to choose among competing duration models:A case study of Hutterite data.In:Cloog C(ed.)Sociological Methodology,1987.American Sociological Association, Washington,DC,pp.247–307
Levy D,Bates D,Caronna J,Cartlidge N,Knill-Jones R, Lapinski R,Singer B,Shaw D,Plum F1981Prognosis in non-traumatic coma.Annals of Internal Medicine94:293–301 Levy D,Caronna J,Singer B,Lapinski R,Frydman H,Plum F 1985Predicting outcome from hypoxic-ischemic coma. Journal of American Medical Association253:1420–26Magnusson D,Bergman L1990A pattern approach to the study of pathways from childhood to adulthood.In:Robins L, Rutter M(eds.)Straight and De ious Pathways from Childhood to Adulthood.Cambridge University Press,Cambridge,UK, pp.101–15
Manton K,Stallard E,Woodbury M,Tolley D,Yashin A1987 Grade-of-membership techniques for studying complex event history processes with unobserved covariates.In:Clogg C (ed.)Sociological Methodology,1987.American Sociological Association,Washington,DC,pp.309–46
Power C,Manor O,Fox A1991Health and Class:The Early Years.Chapman and Hall,London
Ragin C1987The Comparati e Method:Mo ing Beyond Qualitati e and Quantitati e Strategies.University of Cali-fornia Press,Berkeley,CA
Rowe J,Kahn R1998Successful Aging.Pantheon Books,New York
Shifren K,Hooker K,Wood P,Nesselroade J1997Structure and variation of mood in individuals with Parkinson’s disease: A dynamic factor analysis.Psychology and Aging12(2): 328–39
Singer B,RyffC D1997Racial and ethnic inequalities in health: Environmental,psychosocial,and physiological pathways.In: Devlin B,Fienberg S,Resnick D,Roeder K(eds.)Intelligence, Genes,and Success:Scientists Respond to the Bell Cur e, Springer-Verlag(Copernicus),New York,pp.89–122 Singer B,RyffC D2001Person-centered methods for under-standing aging:The integration of numbers and narratives.In: Binstock R,George L K(eds.)Handbook of Aging and the Social Sciences.Academic
Press,San Diego,CA,pp.44–65 Singer B,RyffC,Carr D,Magee W1998Linking life histories and mental health:A person-centered strategy.In:Raftery A (ed.)Sociological Methodology,1998.American Sociological Association,Washington,DC,pp.1–51
Stone L1979The revival of narrative:Reflections on a new old story.Past and Present85:3–24
Turner M1996The Literary Mind.Oxford University Press, New York
Vaillant G1983The Natural History of Alcoholism.Harvard University Press,Cambridge,MA
Wadsworth M,Kuh D1997Childhood influences on adult health:A review of recent work from the British1946national birth cohort study,the MRC national survey of health and development.Pediatric Perinatal Epidemiology11:2–20 Zhang H,Singer B1999Recursi e Partitioning in the Health Sciences.Springer-Verlag,New York
Zhao H,McLanahan S,Brooks-Gunn J,Singer B2000Studying the real child rather than the ideal child:Bringing the person into developmental studies.In:Bergman L,Cairns R,Nilsson L-G,Nystedt L(eds.)De elopmental Science and the Holistic Approach.Lawrence Erlbaum,Mahwah,NJ,pp.393–419
B.H.Singer Longitudinal Data:Event–History Analysis in Discrete Time
The practical motivation for considering discrete-time models of event histories(Allison1982,Thompson 1977)comes from the fact that such longitudinal data
9063
Longitudinal Data:E ent–History Analysis in Discrete Time
Copyright#2001Elsevier Science Ltd.All rights reserved.
International Encyclopedia of the Social&Behavioral Sciences ISBN:0-08-043076-7

版权声明:本站内容均来自互联网,仅供演示用,请勿用于商业和其他非法用途。如果侵犯了您的权益请与我们联系QQ:729038198,我们将在24小时内删除。